Friday 28 September 2012

Mediated Communication: slaying the trolls


I think we could all agree that the internet has changed the way we communicate pretty dramatically compared to 50 years ago.  I mean you have to agree, you’re on a blog!
According to Richard Miller, the internet has brought on “the greatest change in human communication in human history” (2010, p. 143), which has some pretty huge implications for the way we interact with each other and understand the world.  One big thing the internet has changed is that we can interact with others away from the identity ascribed to us in “real life”.  I have generally thought of this as a good thing, for example a recently read an article where a young deaf woman described that that social media was a gateway to interacting with people without prejudice (Hollier 2012).

As it turns out though, there are issues linked with online anonymity. I particularly like the explanation by Randy Foster, managing editor of the New Bern Sun Journal, that “[anonymous] online commenting is the literary equivalent to road rage” (Kabay 2011).  It does make sense, when people are protected by not having to link their actions to their real life identity; they have a lot more confidence, and are often quite mean.
Long-established research in social psychology pointed out that anonymity increases anti-social behaviour.  In a 2008 experiment in Japan, 70 undergraduate students were randomly assigned to four experimental conditions (Anonymous, Nonidentifiable, Nonaccountable, and Nonanonymous) to examine whether they would violate game rules to obtain the monetary reward through anonymity. Only participants in the Anonymous condition violated the rules to obtain the reward" (Kaybay 2011).
In light of recent nasty comments on Twitter people are more upset about the online issue (Jones & Byrnes 2012).  Some newspaper websites have taken to not allowing anonymous comments on their posts (Rosenberry 2011, p. 6).  I completely get where these people are coming from, no-one likes trolls, BUT the internet is a separate place to real life and it allows us to be anyone.  I like the freedom of it, but as the internet is increasingly becoming an integral part of real life it is inevitable it will have to change.  We can’t have trolls roaming the streets, but if we shoo them offline, where will they go next?


Reference List:
Hollier, S 2012, ‘Social media helps find work (and delivers cheap pizza)’, Ramp Up, 23rdMarch, accessed 30/09/2012,http://www.abc.net.au/rampup/articles/2012/03/23/3462327.htm

Jones, G and Byrnes, H 2012, ‘Time is up for Twitter trolls and bullies’, The Daily Telegraph, 11 September, accessed 30/09/2012, http://www.dailytelegraph.com.au/news/time-is-up-for-twitter-trolls-and-bulllies/story-e6freuy9-1226472133504

Kabay, M 2011, ‘See you anon: Reflections on online anonymity’, Network World, 26 Sept accessed 30/09/2012, http://go.galegroup.com.ezproxy.uow.edu.au/ps/i.do?id=GALE%7CA267972186&v=2.1&u=uow&it=r&p=AONE&sw=w

Miller, R 2010, ‘The Coming Apocalypse’, Pedagogy, vol.10, no.1, pp143-151, accessed
Rosenberry, J 2011, 'Users Support Online Anonymity Despite Increasing Negativity', Newspaper Research Journal, vol. 32, no. 2, pp6-19, accessed 30/09/2012, Communication & Mass Media Complete, EBSCOhost.

Thursday 20 September 2012

Bloody Identity Eh?



Language is a hugely important part of forming identity.  As linguist Anna Wierzbicka states, “the way we speak reflects the way we think” (2002, p. 1167). I remember when I started learning German I started to feel a real affiliation with that culture and when I was living in Canada I would either emphasise my Australian accent to assert my identity or would pick up Canadian slang like "eh?' (which they really do say) to fit in with my new friends (convergence and divergence bells ringing anyone?)

After doing some research I confirmed something I already knew.  I speak Standard Australian English, which is the dominant dialect and is used by the vast majority of speakers in Australia. It is a salient marker of national identity, and is used in broadcasting and in public life.  In addition to that Wierzbicka argues that we use the adjective “bloody” a lot.  The Aboriginal and Ethnocultural varieties are minority dialects allowing speakers to express their cultural identity within the multicultural Australian context (Cox & Palethorpe 2007).  I would agree that it is a bit of a quirk of standard Australian English to chuck bloody in a conversation to show you’re not stuck up, because everyone knows Australians do not like tall poppies (Peeters 2004).

The importance of language as a link to and even form of identity can be more obvious when you don’t have it.  In a speech about the importance of Kiah Perry explains that his native language is almost lost and is fighting to save it because language is a part of culture and knowledge as a means of empowering people (Perry 2012, p. 60). He argues it contributes to the well-being of Aboriginal communities, strengthens ties between Elders and young people and gives a sense of identity (Perry 2010, p. 62).  The Department of Aboriginal affairs recognises that “to Aboriginal people, language is much more than just words. It is a direct link to land and country. It holds traditional songs and stories. It is about spirituality and deep meaning, and it reflects unique cultural concepts and ways of looking at the world” (Department of Aboriginal Affairs 2004, p. 1). 

Language is such an important part of identity it would be very painful to lose.  It doesn’t just need to be a national language either (I once heard a great quite that said a language is just a dialect with an army), as regional dialects, accents or even argot can be valued markers of identity.

Reference List:


Cox, F and Palethorpe, S 2007, ‘Australian English’, Journal of the International Phonetic Association, vol. 37, no. 3, pp 341-350, accessed 20/09/2012, http://dx.doi.org.ezproxy.uow.edu.au/10.1017/S0025100307003192

Department of Aboriginal Affairs 2004, New South Wales Aboriginal Languages Policy, NSW Government, NSW, accessed 20/09/2012, http://www.daa.nsw.gov.au/data/files//languagespolicyFINAL.pdf

Peeters, B 2004, ‘‘‘Thou shalt not be a tall poppy’’: Describing an Australian communicative (and behavioral) norm’, Intercultural Pragmatics, vol. 1, no. 1, pp71–92, accessed 27/08/2012, doi: 1612-295X/04/0001–0071

Perry, K 2010, ‘Language and identity: A speech’ Ngoonjook: a Journal of Australian Indigenous Issues, no. 35, pp60-63, accessed 20/09/2012, http://search.informit.com.au.ezproxy.uow.edu.au/documentSummary;dn=880049160454269;res=IELIND
 
Wierzbicka, A 2002, ‘Australian cultural scripts – bloody revisited’, Journal of Pragmatics, no. 34, pp1167-1209.


 

Wednesday 19 September 2012

Commenting on Identity Talk

I commented on Ashleigh Morton's post Idenity Talk.  I'm just posting it here as well in case something terrible happens on this big scary internet.

"I think this topic of codes, semantics and pragmatics is really interesting and I like how your post explores it through a few different areas. It’s so true that our language is overpopulated with the intentions of others and not hard to see why miscommunications can occur in conversations. Everyone brings their own codes to an interaction, so it’s clear to see why communication across any kind of culture (even subcultures like people in jail or who like a certain type of music) can be very tricky! I found your post really helpful in relating this idea of telling to code to a gender-based situation, which is something that always interests me. I have always thought Judith Butler’s ideas about gender as performative have been disturbingly true. I’m only disturbed because there are so many gender-based issues now and throughout history and if it’s all based on talk it’s kind of upsetting! The whole idea that the way we act in any persona we adopt (even for an evening) is informed by the code of the activity and expectations of others. You say that it seems strange that people don’t notice that they’re constantly engaged in this image maintenance, but what happens if we do notice? How would we even deal with that?"

Monday 17 September 2012

Breaching Experiments Presentation

Last Wednesday Ben, Danni and I gave a presentation dressed as a ninja, clown and pirate to explain what is going on with Garinkel, ethnomethodology and breach experiments.  Here it is:


Friday 14 September 2012

Tilt you head and look at the world


The way we interpret the world around us is a pretty interesting topic.  Our good friend Garfinkel states that we have a documentary method of interpretation, which “consists of treating an actual appearance as ‘the document of’, as ‘pointing to’, as ‘standing on behalf of’ a presupposed underlying pattern. Not only is the underlying pattern derived from its individual documentary evidences, but the individual documentary evidences, in their turn, are interpreted on the basis of what is known about the underlying pattern. Each is used to elaborate the other” (2002, p. 78). 
This was a little bit difficult to wrap your head around, so I went trawling through some blogs to see who could explain it.  I found that Mitch SOC250’s blogpost The Social and MoralOrder in Talk used a helpful analogy to explain this concept.  He says that the people we know can be thought of as draws in the filing cabinet that is the brain. When we think about a person e know, that draw is opened and we view the file associated with that person.  These files might be memories, ideas or labels we connect with this person and are based on our experiences with them.
I think that is easy enough to understand.  We build up a repository of information about people (or things) we know, but it is the next part of the analogy that gets interesting.
The documenting system works in a cyclic fashion, yes that’s right.  We develop an image of who people are, and so this image dictates how we perceive that person.  For example if we think that someone is an intelligent person, if we see them silent when faced with a question we might assume they are pondering an eloquent answer or giving silence as a clever response rather than not knowing the answer.  We try to fit things into to our understanding of the world rather than change it. So essentially individual evidences are interpreted on the basis of what is already known about the underlying pattern (Heritage 1984, p. 86). 
To illustrate this I thought it would be fun to fill this post with as many duck-rabbits as I could find.  If what is known about the following images is that they are ducks, then all information that does not support their duckness is treated as irrelevant.  Now watch what happens when you think they’re rabbits…


http://emscheffel.wordpress.com/2009/05/17/turbulent-times-and-wittgensteins-duckrabbit/
http://neurochannels.blogspot.com.au/2009_09_01_archive.html

http://www.bunnylicious.org/2009/01/perceptual-interpretation/
http://forums.philosophyforums.com/threads/wittgenstein-in-a-duckrabbit-47554.html
http://duckrabbit.blogspot.com.au/

Reference List:

Garfinkel 2002, Studies  in  Ethnomethodology , Polity Press, Cambridge.

Heritage, J. 1984, ‘The Morality of Cognition’, in Garfinkel and ethnomethodology, Polity Press, Cambridge, pp75-102.

Tuesday 4 September 2012

Breaking the Rules of Reality

Garfinkel's breaching experiments are pretty famous in the sociology world, but what exactly are these “breaching experiments”?  Essentially, Harold Garfinkel was a pretty cool sociologist guy who developed endomethodology as a field of inquiry in sociology.  Garfinkel was interested in how social structures are ordinarily and routinely maintained, which most sociologists would examine by selecting some set of stable features of an organisation of activities and look for variables that contribute to their stability.  Garfinkel thought it would be more economical to start with a situation of stable features and ask what can be done to make for trouble (Heritage 1984, p. 78).
Garfinkel decided to conduct a series of breaching experiments, where he broke the normal rules of a situation and wanted to see create a senseless situation.  The simplest experiment was where the experimenter played a game of ticktaktoe (noughts and crosses) with a subject.  The subject made the first move, and then the experimenter erased the subjects first move, moved it to another cell and marked his own, without ever indicating this was unusual behaviour.  There were 253 experimental trials and in 95% of these, the subjects reacted to this action in some way.  75% of subjects objected or demanded an explanation and the rest assumed there was some sort of new game in progress and adapted to the new “rules” or assumed it was a practical joke.  Essentially, the subjects abandoned the “noughts and crosses” framework and adopted a new interpretive framework to make sense of the situation.  In contrast, people who held on to the idea that noughts and crosses was still in play were pretty disturbed.
From this experiment Garfinkel made two significant conclusions; one that when there were behaviours that didn’t comply with the basic rules of the game people tried to treat it as a legally possible event.  The other conclusion he made is that disturbance was increased if people tried to keep the original set of rules.
So this is pretty interesting, and Garfinkel though so too, but he didn’t think he could take his experiments into real life situations.  Games have obvious rules that can be followed or broken, and life is a lot more complicated.  Also, breaking rules in real life might have more serious consequences.
So he didn’t get his hands dirty, Garfinkel got his students to do the experiments.  The most famous experiments, and the ones mentioned in the reading, are when Garfinkel requested his students engage an acquaintance or friend in an ordinary conversation and, without indicating what the experimenter was saying was in any way out of the ordinary, to insist that the personal clarify the sense of his commonplace remarks (Garfinkel 1963, p. 221).
One of my favourite examples is when an experimenter was in a carpool and the subject said they had a flat tyre.  With the simple question of, “What do you mean a flat tyre?” The subject got really upset, saying “What do you mean? What do you mean? A flat tyre is a flat tyre.  That is what I meant.  Nothing special.  What a crazy question!”
Just looking at this experiment it is clear that the interaction broke down incredibly quickly, apparently even Garfinkel was surprised and how rapid and complete the breakdowns were (Heritage 1984, p. 81).  This breakdown was not just because the subject didn’t want to answer the experimenter’s question; it was because if we can’t bring background knowledge to a situation and obey conversational rules, then mutual understanding and the entire shared experience of life could collapse at any moment (Heritage 1984, p. 95)!
As much as people were very upset at the slightest breaking of perceived normality, Garfinkel couldn’t create a situation that was utterly senseless.  People seemed to be less distressed, indignant and less likely to demand an explanation if they could make sense of the situation in some way (Heritage 1984, p. 83).  So Garfinkel found that all actions, whether in a simple game or a social interaction, are perceived to have a constitutive structure and a threat to the normal order of these events is upsetting because if we don’t behave according to the rules social organisation simply disintegrates (Garfinkel 1963, p. 198).
After some YouTube searching, it seems like performing breaching experiments is a pretty common task for sociology students and people who enjoy creating a little chaos.  So I’ll show you two examples that I thought illustrated the idea of what a breaching experiment is quite nicely.  

In this clip it’s interesting that the lecturer and the audience respond by laughing, which suggests they adopted a framework of thinking that this person is doing flips for their entertainment.  They don’t descend into chaos or assume that it was normal lecture behaviour.

This clip is a fantastic breach of normal behaviour as it’s almost as simple as the noughts and crosses example.  Instead of going down the escalator, the person runs up it.  You probably noticed in this clip too that people didn’t all start running up or go crazy, they just moved to the side and probably assumed the girl was playing a joke of some kind rather than believe a world eists where the down escalator wasn’t down any more.

Another famous breaching experiment was conducted by Stanley Milgram in the 1970s.  Milgram instructed students to ride the New York Subway and ask people to give them their seats.  This experiment was interesting, because not only did many people give up their seats when someone asked with no justification, the students experimenting found the task emotionally very difficult (Luo 2004).  In an article on the experiment in the New York Times, interviews with the students many years later show that they found the experiment traumatic and some were physically sick at the idea of violating the rules of behaviour expected on the subway.  This is very interesting as it suggests that breaking the perceived normality is very distressing even when you do have the framework to know what is going on.  It’s important to note that although both experimenters and subjects in the breaching experiments got upset, angry or even felt sick, there was never a situation where people just decided, “OK, this makes no sense and I can’t comprehend this.”


Reference List:
Garfinkel, H. 1963 ‘A Conception of and Experiments with ‘Trust’ as a Condition of Concerted Stable Actions’ in O’Brien, J (ed.) The Production of Reality: Essays and Readings on Social Interaction, vol. 4, California, Pine Forge Press, pp370-81
Heritage, J. 1984, ‘The Morality of Cognition’, in Garfinkel and ethnomethodology, Polity Press, Cambridge, pp75-102.
Luo, M 2004, ‘Excuse Me. May I Have Your Seat?'; Revisiting a Social Experiment, And the Fear That Goes With It’, New York Times, 14 September, accessed 02/09/2012, http://select.nytimes.com/gst/abstract.html?res=F20F15FE3A540C778DDDA00894DC404482